particle filtering
play

Particle Filtering Sometimes |X| is too big to use exact inference - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Particle Filtering Sometimes |X| is too big to use exact inference |X| may be too big to even store B(X) E.g. X is continuous |X| 2 may be too big to do updates Solution: approximate inference Track samples of X, not


  1. Particle Filtering Ø Sometimes |X| is too big to use exact inference • |X| may be too big to even store B(X) • E.g. X is continuous |X| 2 may be too big to do updates • Ø Solution: approximate inference • Track samples of X, not all values • Samples are called particles • Time per step is linear in the number of samples • But: number needed may be large • In memory: list of particles Ø This is how robot localization works in practice 1

  2. Forward algorithm vs. particle filtering Forward algorithm Particle filtering Ø Elapse of time • Elapse of time B’(X t )= Σ x t-1 p(X t |x t-1 )B(x t-1 ) x--->x’ • Observe Ø Observe w(x’)=p(e t |x) B(X t ) ∝ p(e t |X t )B’(X t ) • Resample Ø Renormalize resample N particles B(x t ) sum up to 1 2

  3. Today Ø Speech recognition • A massive HMM! Ø Introduction to machine learning 3

  4. Speech and Language Ø Speech technologies • Automatic speech recognition (ASR) • Text-to-speech synthesis (TTS) • Dialog systems Ø Language processing technologies • Machine translation • Information extraction • Web search, question answering • Text classification, spam filtering, etc… 4

  5. Digitizing Speech 5

  6. The Input Ø Speech input is an acoustic wave form Graphs from Simon Arnfield’s web tutorial on speech, sheffield: http://www.psyc.leeds.ac.uk/research/cogn/speech/tutorial/ 6

  7. 7

  8. The Input Ø Frequency gives pitch; amplitude gives volume • Sampling at ~8 kHz phone, ~16 kHz mic Ø Fourier transform of wave displayed as a spectrogram • Darkness indicates energy at each frequency 8

  9. Acoustic Feature Sequence Ø Time slices are translated into acoustic feature vectors (~39 real numbers per slice) Ø These are the observations, now we need the hidden states X 9

  10. State Space Ø p(E|X) encodes which acoustic vectors are appropriate for each phoneme (each kind of sound) Ø p(X|X’) encodes how sounds can be strung together Ø We will have one state for each sound in each word Ø From some state x, can only: • Stay in the same state (e.g. speaking slowly) • Move to the next position in the word • At the end of the word, move to the start of the next word Ø We build a little state graph for each word and chain them together to form our state space X 10

  11. HMMs for Speech 11

  12. Transitions with Bigrams 12

  13. Decoding Ø While there are some practical issues, finding the words given the acoustics is an HMM inference problem Ø We want to know which state sequence x 1:T is most likely given the evidence e 1:T : * x argmax p x | e ( ) = 1: T 1: T 1: T x 1: T argmax p x , e ( ) = 1: T 1: T x 1: T Ø From the sequence x, we can simply read off the words 13

  14. Machine Learning Ø Up until now: how to reason in a model and how to make optimal decisions Ø Machine learning: how to acquire a model on the basis of data / experience • Learning parameters (e.g. probabilities) • Learning structure (e.g. BN graphs) • Learning hidden concepts (e.g. clustering) 14

  15. Parameter Estimation Ø Estimating the distribution of a random variable Ø Elicitation: ask a human Ø Empirically: use training data (learning!) • E.g.: for each outcome x, look at the empirical rate of that value: count x ( ) p x = ( ) ML p r = 1 3 ( ) total samples ML • This is the estimate that maximizes the likelihood of the data L x , p x ( ) ( ) θ = ∏ i θ i 15

  16. Estimation: Smoothing Ø Relative frequencies are the maximum likelihood estimates (MLEs) count x argmax p X | ( ) ( ) θ = θ ML p x = ( ) θ ML total samples argmax p X ( ) ∏ = i θ θ i Ø In Bayesian statistics, we think of the parameters as just another random variable, with its own distribution argmax p | X ( ) θ = θ MAP θ argmax p X | p p X ???? ( ) ( ) ( ) = θ θ θ argmax p X | p ( ) ( ) = θ θ 16 θ

  17. Estimation: Laplace Smoothing Ø Laplace’s estimate: • Pretend you saw every outcome once more than you actually did ( ) + 1 c x ( ) = p LAP x ! # ( ) + 1 ∑ c x " $ p X ( ) = x ML ( ) + 1 c x = p X ( ) = N + X LAP 17

  18. Estimation: Laplace Smoothing Ø Laplace’s estimate (extended): • Pretend you saw every outcome k extra times c x k ( ) + p x = ( ) LAP k , N k X + p X ( ) = • What’s Laplace with k=0? LAP ,0 • k is the strength of the prior p X ( ) = LAP ,1 Ø Laplace for conditionals: • Smooth each condition p X ( ) = LAP ,100 independently: c x y , k ( ) + p x y | = ( ) LAP k , c y k X ( ) + 18

  19. Example: Spam Filter Ø Input: email Ø Output: spam/ham Ø Setup: • Get a large collection of example emails, each labeled “spam” or “ham” • Note: someone has to hand label all this data! • Want to learn to predict labels of new, future emails Ø Features: the attributes used to make the ham / spam decision • Words: FREE! • Text patterns: $dd, CAPS • Non-text: senderInContacts • …… 19

  20. Example: Digit Recognition Ø Input: images / pixel grids Ø Output: a digit 0-9 Ø Setup: • Get a large collection of example images, each labeled with a digit • Note: someone has to hand label all this data! • Want to learn to predict labels of new, future digit images Ø Features: the attributes used to make the digit decision • Pixels: (6,8) = ON • Shape patterns: NumComponents, AspectRation, NumLoops • …… 20

  21. A Digit Recognizer Ø Input: pixel grids Ø Output: a digit 0-9 21

  22. Naive Bayes for Digits Ø Simple version: • One feature F ij for each grid position <i,j> • Possible feature values are on / off, based on whether intensity is more or less than 0.5 in underlying image • Each input maps to a feature vector, e.g. → F 0,0 = 0 F 0,1 = 0 F 0,2 = 1 F 0,3 = 1 F 0,4 = 0  F 15,15 = 0 • Here: lots of features, each is binary valued Ø Naive Bayes model: ( ) ( ) ∝ p Y ( ) p Y | F 0,0  F ∏ p F i , j | Y 15,15 i , j Ø What do we need to learn? 22

  23. General Naive Bayes Ø A general naive Bayes model: n Y × F parameters ( ) = p Y , F 1  F n ( ) ( ) p Y ∏ p F i | Y i Y parameters n × Y × F parameters Ø We only specify how each feature depends on the class Ø Total number of parameters is linear in n 23

  24. Inference for Naive Bayes Ø Goal: compute posterior over causes • Step 1: get joint probability of causes and evidence ( ) = p Y , f 1  f n " % ( ) ( ) p y 1 p f i | c 1 ∏ $ ' ! $ ( ) p y 1 , f 1  f n i $ ' # & ( ) ( ) p y 2 ∏ p f i | c 2 $ ' # & $ ' ( ) p y 2 , f 1  f n i $ ' # &  $ ' # &  ( ) ( ) p y k ∏ p f i | c k $ ' # & $ ' # & i ( ) p y k , f 1  f n # & ( ) " % p f 1  f n • Step 2: get probability of evidence • Step 3: renormalize ( ) p Y | f 1  f n 24

  25. General Naive Bayes Ø What do we need in order to use naive Bayes? • Inference (you know this part) • Start with a bunch of conditionals, p(Y) and the p(F i |Y) tables • Use standard inference to compute p(Y|F 1 …F n ) • Nothing new here • Estimates of local conditional probability tables • p(Y), the prior over labels • p(F i |Y) for each feature (evidence variable) • These probabilities are collectively called the parameters of the model and denoted by θ • Up until now, we assumed these appeared by magic, but… • … they typically come from training data: we’ll look at this now 25

  26. Examples: CPTs p Y ( ) ( ) ( ) p F on Y | p F on Y | = = 3,1 5,5 1 0.1 1 0.01 1 0.05 2 0.1 2 0.05 2 0.01 3 0.1 3 0.05 3 0.90 4 0.1 4 0.30 4 0.80 5 0.1 5 0.80 5 0.90 6 0.1 6 0.90 6 0.90 7 0.1 7 0.05 7 0.25 8 0.1 8 0.60 8 0.85 9 0.1 9 0.50 9 0.60 0 0.1 0 0.80 0 0.80 26

  27. Important Concepts Ø Data: labeled instances, e.g. emails marked spam/ham • Training set • Held out set • Test set Ø Features: attribute-value pairs which characterize each x Ø Experimentation cycle • Learn parameters (e.g. model probabilities) on training set • (Tune hyperparameters on held-out set) • Compute accuracy of test set • Very important: never “peek” at the test set! Ø Evaluation • Accuracy: fraction of instances predicted correctly Ø Overfitting and generalization • Want a classifier which does well on test data • Overfitting: fitting the training data very closely, but not generalizing well 27

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend