variation and mental
play

Variation and mental representation Gregory R. Guy New York - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Variation and mental representation Gregory R. Guy New York University Words in the mind Abstract representations, composed of strings of phonemes. Morphological structure either stored in representation or generated by derivation


  1. Variation and mental representation Gregory R. Guy New York University

  2. Words in the mind • Abstract representations, composed of strings of phonemes. • Morphological structure either stored in representation or generated by derivation and /ænd/ band /bænd/ banned /bæn#d/

  3. Words in speech • Surface forms reflect allophony, phrasal phonology, etc. • Show variable realizations reflecting variable phonological processes. – e.g., final coronal stop deletion and ~ an’ band ~ ban’ banned ~ ban’

  4. What’s in the mental lexicon? Variable processes are conditioned by: • Lexical frequency • Morphological structure • Lexical exceptions • Priming

  5. I. Frequency effects on variation • coronal stop deletion in English • -ing/-in alternation in English • coda – s lenition in Caribbean Spanish

  6. Coronal stop deletion increases with lexical frequency Guy, Hay & Walker 2008. p=.0005

  7. -ing > -in: More frequent words show more alveolar [ɪn] pronunciation Figure 1. [ ɪn ] by lexical frequency 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 p [ ɪn] 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 <8 9-10 11 12+ Source: Laturnus, log10 freq de Vilchez, Chaves & Guy, 2016.

  8. Spanish -s lenition (Erker 2008) No significant frequency effect • Correlation between frequency and: – Spectral center of gravity N=318 r = -.02 p = .74, n.s. – Duration N=453 r = .07 p = .136, n.s.

  9. Bybee on lexical frequency effects (Bybee 2000) • “ The more a word is used, the more it is exposed to the reductive effect of articulatory automation … ” • “ Sound change affects stored representations incrementally each time a word is used … ”

  10. 2. Morphological constraints on variation • coda – s deletion in Brazilian Portuguese • -ing/-in alternation in English • coronal stop deletion in English

  11. Morphological constraint on coda /s/ deletion in Brazilian Portuguese (Guy 1981) Coda /s/ deletion in unstressed final syllables N % deleted Monomorphemes 1392 53% e.g. menos Nominal plurals * 5247 5% e.g. eles, todos *NB: first position in NP only

  12. Morphological constraint on – in/-ing alternation in English Laturnus et al. 2016 N % [n] weight Lexical class verbs 489 55% .53 something, 81 67% .51 nothing nouns 79 38% .32 12

  13. Morphological constraint on coronal stop deletion in English N % deleted Monomorphemes 658 38.1% e.g., guest Irregular Past 56 33.9 e.g., lost Regular Past 181 16.0 e.g., guessed (Guy 1992 corpus)

  14. Morphological structure interacts with lexical frequency: coronal stop deletion (Myers & Guy 1997) Monomorphemic words Significant frequency effect N Deletions % Del Low frequency 151 28 18.5 High frequency 573 194 33.9 p < .01

  15. Morphology interacts with frequency: coronal stop deletion (Myers & Guy 1997) Regular Past Tense Verbs No significant frequency effect N Deletions % Del Low frequency 96 7 7.3 High frequency 220 18 8.2 chi-sq (1df) = .073, p > .70

  16. Morphology interacts with frequency: coronal stop deletion (Myers & Guy 1997) Frequency effect by morphology 40 35 30 % -t,d deletion 25 monomorphemes 20 regular past 15 10 5 0 low freq high freq Lexical frequency

  17. -t,d morphology: Fruehwald (probabilities of /t,d/ retention)

  18. Variation and morphology: conclusions • Variable processes are sensitive to morphological structure; they can be conditioned by derived environments. • Hence variable phonological processes ‘see’ mental representations that incorporate morphological information, in addition to the phonological content. • Morphological constraints on phonology interact with lexical frequency: derived forms are less affected by frequency

  19. Morphology and lexical frequency • Bybee’s model of lenition fed by frequency is phonetically motivated • This should be independent of and orthogonal to morphological constraints • But other models make other predictions. Pinker and others argue that regular derived forms are generated by rule; only roots and irregular forms are stored • Hence, regular derived forms lack independent mental representations to accumulate frequency or collocational information

  20. 3. Lexical exceptions • Coronal stop deletion in English: exceptional and • Coda /s/ deletion in Brazilian Portuguese: exceptional 1pl morpheme – mos • Coda /s/ deletion in Salvadoran Spanish: exceptional entonces, digamos • /ay/ monophthongization in Southern US English: exceptional I, my

  21. Lexical exceptions in variation Many variable processes are known to exhibit unusually high rates of occurrence in particular lexical items. -e.g., coronal stop deletion in English is exceptionally frequent in ‘ and ’ (Exceptional because deletion occurs significantly more often in and than in phonologically comparable words like sand, band, hand , etc.)

  22. Exceptional and in the NZE corpus: N Deletion rate and 597 80% other words 3348 29%

  23. Lexical exceptions are not just high frequency words • The second-highest frequency word in the ONZE corpus was just ; it showed significant following context effects and did not behave like and • Spanish menos is higher in frequency than entonces and digamos , but does not behave exceptionally

  24. Following context effect in the NZE corpus Other words and Following N % del N % del Context: __C 1339 58.3 315 87.9 __V 1477 10.4 182 75.3 Range: 47.9% > 12.6% ( 18 speakers from the ONZE corpus at U Canterbury)

  25. Contextual effects on Brazilian Portuguese – s deletion • verbs ending in 1plural – mos suffix omit final /s/ at exceptionally high rate. • Coda /s/ deletion mainly occurs in preconsonantal position, and is strongly constrained by place manner and voicing of the following C • Do these constraints affect exceptional – mos words just like other words?

  26. Lexical exceptions in Brazilian Portuguese: coda -s deletion [factor weights] Features of following C Non-exceptions Lexical exceptions (- mos forms) Voice/Manner: sonorant .69 .49 voiced obstruent .44 .58 voiceless obstruent .36 .44 Range .33 > .14 Place: labial .32 .58 coronal .61 .53 velar .44 .39 Range .29 > .19 N: 5880 1225 Goodness of fit (log likelihood) -704.8 -791.5

  27. Following context effect appears significantly weaker in exceptional -mos • Range of probabilities is smaller for both the place effect and the manner/voicing effect • The goodness of fit measure is significantly worse for the exceptional forms, suggesting that they aren’t as well explained by the contextual conditions

  28. Contextual effects on Salvadoran Spanish – s deletion • El Salvador has variable final – s deletion, like other Caribbean dialects • Hoffman 2004 finds strong constraint effects on deletion; more deletion in stressed syllables, more deletion before consonants, than before vowels • Three discourse markers show exceptionally high rates of deletion: entonces, digamos, pues

  29. -s deletion in Salvadoran Spanish (Hoffman 2004) Non-exceptional words Lexical exceptions Following context: ( entonces, digamos, pues ) sonorant .60 .63 voiced obstruent .75 .55 voiceless obstruent .33 .38 vowel .36 .38 pause .44 .56 Range .42 > .25 Syllable Stress: stressed .38 .42 unstressed .62 .58 Range .24 > .16

  30. Another variable: monophthongal /ay/ in Southern American English (SoAmEng) • The English diphthong /ay/ is variably monophthongized to /a/ in Southern American English • More monophthongs are found in pre-voiced contexts ( ride vs. right ), in phonetically shorter syllables, and among lower status speakers • I and my are lexical exceptions, with very high rates of monophthongization, even before voiceless consonants (cf: ‘my time’ )

  31. /ay/ monophthongization in SoAmEng: following context effect (Woods 2008) Other words I, my % monophthong 34% 53% Fol. Context: __C[+vce] .76 (.51) __V or G .41 (.49) __C[-vce] .17 (.48) Range: .59 > .03 (n.s.)

  32. /ay/ monophthongization in Southern AmEng: duration effect Other words I, my Duration: shorter .89 .68 longer .49 .45 Range: .40 > .23 (Data from Woods 2008)

  33. Contextual effects are much weaker on exceptional I, my in SoAmEng • Following context effect is not significant for I, my • Duration effect is much weaker • Monophthongization occurs much more often in these two words, and is relatively insensitive to context.

  34. Conclusion: Exceptional words have alternate mental representations • Lexical exceptions to variable processes reveal allomorphy in the mental representation • They have additional lexical entries that incorporate the output of the variable process. – and ~ an’ or ‘n’ - I, my ~ [a], [ma] – -mos ~ -mo – - entonces, digamos ~ entonce, digamo • When the ‘reduced’ allomorph is selected, context has no effect, and apparent frequency of occurrence of the variable phonological process is increased.

  35. 4. Priming • Specific variants tend to occur in clusters: e.g., in BP – s deletion, one deleted form is likely to favor subsequent deleted forms, an undeleted form favors subsequent undeleted forms.

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend