nasals approximants rhotics and laterals
play

Nasals, approximants, rhotics, and laterals C s m v hc v ng m hc - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Nasals, approximants, rhotics, and laterals C s m v hc v ng m hc Formant ranges, F1 and F2: F1: 300 1000 Hz Lecture 15 F2: 800 2500 Hz Recap Recap Last time we saw that stops consist of both a closure


  1. Nasals, approximants, rhotics, and laterals Cơ sở âm vị học và ngữ âm học Formant ranges, F1 and F2: F1: 300 ∼ 1000 Hz Lecture 15 F2: 800 ∼ 2500 Hz Recap Recap • Last time we saw that stops consist of both a closure • Voiceless stops can be identified by concentrations of ( cấu âm tắc ) and a burst ( tiếng nổ ). energy ( khu vực năng lượng ) in the release burst. 5000 k p t Frequency 0 0 0.738 From Kent & Read (1992), The Acoustic Analysis of Speech Time (s)

  2. Recap Nasals • Like stops, nasals involve a vocal tract constriction ( cấu âm thắt trong đường dẫn âm )... • Voiced stops, on the other hand, were better identified using formant transitions ( chuyển tiếp formant ). simmer http://www.cns.nyu.edu/ ∼ david/courses/perception/lecturenotes/speech/speech.html From Ladefoged (2003), Phonetic Data Analysis Nasals Nasals • ...but like vowels, nasals have a periodic glottal source • Energy (amplitude) is lower than for vowels – in part ( nguồn thanh hầu có chu kỳ ). because nasal membranes absorb the sound (màng nhầy hút âm thanh). simmer From Ladefoged (2003), Phonetic Data Analysis

  3. Nasals Nasals • Like stops, the crucial information is contained in the • Since the oral tract is completely closed, this effect is formant transitions. enhanced, giving rise to antiformants ( formant yếu ). • In practice, the velar nasal is usually the clearest. dimmer dinner dinger Nasals Formant transitions preceding nasals • Like stops, the crucial information is contained in the formant transitions. − [m] : falling F2 ← • In practice, the velar nasal is usually the clearest. − [n] : level F2 ← − [N] : rising F2 ← dimmer dinner dinger (reverse for transitions following nasals) From Ladefoged (2003), Phonetic Data Analysis

  4. Nasals Approximants • F1 tends to be low (250-300Hz) and F2 around 2500Hz. In between (where vowels have F2) there is little energy. • Vowels are periodic – complex repeating waves ama ana a N a generated by regular vibrations of the vocal folds. • Consonants may have an aperiodic component, but can still be voiced or voiceless • There are also (largely periodic) segments that lie somewhere in between: approximants. Approximants Approximants • Vowels are periodic – complex repeating waves • Vowels are periodic – complex repeating waves generated by regular vibrations of the vocal folds. generated by regular vibrations of the vocal folds. • Consonants may have an aperiodic component, • Consonants may have an aperiodic component, but can still be voiced or voiceless but can still be voiced or voiceless • There are also (largely periodic) segments that lie • There are also (largely periodic) segments that lie somewhere in between: approximants. somewhere in between: approximants.

  5. Approximants Approximants • Acoustically, [j] [w] are very similar to vowels, with • Acoustically, [j] [w] are very similar to vowels, with striations and formants, but often with less energy striations and formants, but often with less energy • For this reason they are sometimes called semivowels . • For this reason they are sometimes called semivowels . • The constriction for [j] [w] is narrower than for [i] [u], but • The constriction for [j] [w] is narrower than for [i] [u], but formant values are similar formant values are similar Approximants Approximants • Acoustically, [j] [w] are very similar to vowels, with striations and formants, but often with less energy • For this reason they are sometimes called semivowels . • The constriction for [j] [w] is narrower than for [i] [u], but formant values are similar From Borden, Harris, & Raphael (1994)

  6. Approximants Approximants • [w] and [j] tend to differ from [u] and [i] in that they tend • What constitutes a diphthong vs. a semivowel + a vowel not to have a steady-state portion ( một phần không đổi ). can be controversial... From Kent & Read (1992), The Acoustic Analysis of Speech From Kent & Read (1992), The Acoustic Analysis of Speech Rhotics (‘r’-tính) Rhotics (‘r’-tính) • The defining acoustic signature of [ô] is the third formant • The defining acoustic signature of [ô] is the third formant (F3) making a steep fall to below 2kHz. (F3) making a steep fall to below 2kHz. • This effect is referred to as ‘r-colouring’ ( r-hoá ). • This effect is referred to as ‘r-colouring’ ( r-hoá ). • [ô] – and other rhotics ( [r], [R], [ö] ) – are the only • [ô] – and other rhotics ( [r], [R], [ö] ) – are the only approximants for which F3 is (perceptually) important approximants for which F3 is (perceptually) important

  7. Tongue configuration types of Rhotics (‘r’-tính) American English /r/ ([ ô ]) • The defining acoustic signature of [ô] is the third formant (F3) making a steep fall to below 2kHz. • This effect is referred to as ‘r-colouring’ ( r-hoá ). • [ô] – and other rhotics ( [r], [R], [ö] ) – are the only approximants for which F3 is (perceptually) important Examples of corresponding American English tongue configuration types for sustained /r/ as identified from MRI by Tiede et al. (2004). Adapted from Tiede et al. (2007), ‘Variability of North American English /r/ Production in Response to Palatal Perturbation’, Haskins Internal Workshop on Speech Production and Motor Control. Rhoticization Taps ( âm vỗ ) and trills ( âm rung ) 3: Ç: From Ladefoged (2003), Phonetic Data Analysis

  8. Where F3 is useful Lateral approximants • [l]: faint formants at a) low frequency and b) about • [ô] : characterised by low F2 and especially low F3. 1000-1500 and 2500-3000 Hz, followed by abrupt change in amplitude when the tongue tip breaks away. Frequency (Hz) Time (ms) From Ladefoged (2006), A Course In Phonetics From Ladefoged (2006), A Course In Phonetics Clear vs. dark [l] Clear vs. dark [l] • Catalonian (clear, top) vs. Mallorcan (dark, bottom) /l/s 5000 [ l ] • Allophones in many F2 English dialects: dark /l/ 5000 [ l ] in coda (‘feel’), clear /l/ F2 Frequency (Hz) in onset (‘leaf’) Frequency (Hz) 0 • In Newcastle English, 5000 initial /l/ is light; in [ ɫ ] 0 Recasens & Espinosa (2005), ‘Articulatory, Leeds, initial /l/ is dark F2 positional and coarticulatory characteristics for 5000 clear /l/ and dark /l/: evidence from two Catalan (Carter & Local, 2007) [ ɫ ] dialects’, JIPA 35(1), 1-25. F2 • In Scottish English, /l/ can be dark in both 0 Time positions. 0 Time

  9. Clear vs. dark [l] Clear vs. dark [l] 5000 5000 [ l ] [ l ] • Allophones in many • Allophones in many F2 F2 English dialects: dark /l/ English dialects: dark /l/ in coda (‘feel’), clear /l/ in coda (‘feel’), clear /l/ Frequency (Hz) Frequency (Hz) in onset (‘leaf’) in onset (‘leaf’) 0 0 • In Newcastle English, • In Newcastle English, 5000 5000 initial /l/ is light; in initial /l/ is light; in [ ɫ ] [ ɫ ] Leeds, initial /l/ is dark Leeds, initial /l/ is dark F2 F2 (Carter & Local, 2007) (Carter & Local, 2007) • In Scottish English, /l/ • In Scottish English, /l/ can be dark in both can be dark in both 0 0 Time positions. Time positions. Spectrogram practice • What distinguishes these approximants?

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend