hedonic g glms a n new scale t that pe permits valid
play

Hedonic g gLMS: a n new scale t that pe permits valid hedonic c - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

International Citrus & Beverage Conference (ICBC) September 14-17, 2010 Hedonic g gLMS: a n new scale t that pe permits valid hedonic c comparisons Linda Bartoshuk Smell & Taste Center lbartoshuk@dental.ufl.edu 352-273-5119


  1. International Citrus & Beverage Conference (ICBC) September 14-17, 2010 Hedonic g gLMS: a n new scale t that pe permits valid hedonic c comparisons Linda Bartoshuk Smell & Taste Center lbartoshuk@dental.ufl.edu 352-273-5119

  2. Outline of talk 1. Introduction to supertasters 2. Problem: Conventional scales make invalid comparisons across subjects (i.e., scales cannot “see” supertasters). 3. Solution: It’s not easy, but valid comparisons are possible.

  3. Do we all live in the same taste worlds? NO NO Super ertaster ers experience more intense oral sensations than the rest of us.

  4. PROP DEMONSTRATION • PROP is a medication used to suppress thyroid function in patient’s with Grave’s disease. Typical dose: 200 mg/day. • PROP paper: about 1.6 mg of PROP

  5. This tongue has been swabbed with blue food coloring. The structures that house taste buds (fungiform papillae do not stain as well as the rest of the tongue so they appear lighter. Keep a mental picture of this tongue.

  6. This is the tongue of a su super ertast ster er: a person born with an unusually large number of fungiform papillae.

  7. • We count the papillae in a circular template 6 mm in diameter just to the side of the midline.

  8. Supertasters can have up to about 60 fungiform papillae in the template area. Most individuals have far fewer. This individual has 16 fungiform papillae in the template area. Values as low as 5 are normal.

  9. TA TASTE: Supertasters have the most fungiform papillae (the structures that house tastebuds) so they have the most tastebuds and thus perceive the most intense tastes. 50 Suc ucros ose QHC QHCl r=.46, p p<.0001 r=.62, p p<.0001 40 30 ate ones) es) 20 Estimat y nsity o ton 10 to t Intens ude Es lized t 0 Taste Int ormalized agnitud 50 Cit itric ic A Acid id Tas Magn NaCl (r=.22, p p<.05) (nor (r=.49, p p<.001) 40 (n 30 20 10 0 0 10 20 30 40 0 10 20 30 40 Magn agnitud ude E Estimat ate Bit Bitter erness o ess of . f .0032 M 0032 M P PROP (no norm rmal alized t d to t tone ones)

  10. ORAL BURN: Tastebuds are surrounded by fibers thought to mediate oral burn; thus supertasters perceive greater burn from oral irritants like chilis. 100 ppm c 100 ppm caps psaicin Jalapeno peppers contain 600 600 r = = . .56, 56, p < p < . .0001 0001 about 100 ppm capsaicin. 500 500 urn 400 400 bur al b 300 300 Oral Or 200 200 100 100 0 0 100 100 200 200 300 300 Bitter Bit erness o ess of . f .0032 M 0032 M P PROP Snyder Senior Essay, Yale University, 1996

  11. ORAL T TOUCH: Fungiform papillae are innervated by fibers mediating touch; thus supertasters perceive more intense touch sensations (e.g., creaminess) from fats 10 100 hea eavy cr crea eam 90 90 r = = .44, p < .44, p < .001 .001 80 80 ess 70 70 Creamines 60 60 50 40 30 20 20 10 10 0 NT M MT S ST Duffy, Bartoshuk, Lucchina, Snyder &, Tym, 1996.

  12. In addition, there is a connection between supertasting and flavor (flavor is taste plus RETRONASAL OLFACTION)

  13. Food and Beverage Industry • Has long known that adding a taste to a beverage (e.g., adding sugar) will intensify the perception of the flavor (e.g., orange) of the beverage. • Similarly, experiencing greater taste (as do su supertast sters) will intensify the perception of flavor.

  14. Nontasters Orthonasal Retronasal Supertasters Orthonasal Retronasal

  15. • Supertasters live in a neon eon food world. • Those with the fewest taste buds live in a el food world. pa pastel

  16. I’ve compared supertasters to others with regard to how they perceive the food world. But we cannot share each other’s experiences directly. How did I make comparisons between supertasters and others?

  17. I did not ot use the classic Natick 9-point scales (or any similar scales like VAS).

  18. Natick 9-point scales Sensory Hedonic like extremely extremely strong 9 9 8 8 7 7 6 6 neutral 5 5 4 4 3 3 2 2 extremely weak dislike extremely 1 1

  19. The problem. • What does “ext extrem emel ely strong” mean? • What does “like ext extrem emel ely” mean? • Let me begin with the sensory descriptor: “ext extrem emel ely strong.”

  20. • In ordinary conversation, we use intensity descriptors to communicate with each other and compare experiences. “That lemonade tastes ex extrem emel ely st strong to me. Does it taste ex extrem emel ely st strong to you?” • Labeled scales use those same descriptors to denote perceived intensities. • We seem to be communicating, but are we? • No! “ex extrem emel ely st strong ” ” may denote different perceived taste intensities to each of us.

  21. • How do our subjects interpret “ ex extrem emel ely stro rong ng” ” on the 9-point scale? • Do they tend to interpret “ ex extrem emel ely str strong “ as referring only to foods when they are in a food experiment? • Let’s ask them.

  22. We asked our subjects to rate their favorite and least favorite foods. • The median for favorite favorite food was “9.” like extremely 9 food 8 • The median for least 7 favorite food was “1.” 6 neutral 5 • This tells us that the 4 Natick 9-point scale was 3 interpreted as referring to food preferences only. 2 least฀ 1 dislike extremely favorite food

  23. But consider how easily we switch contexts for ratings.

  24. Example: elastic intensity scale • A woman who has just experienced childbirth may describe her pain as “ve very st y stron ong. g.” • Given a cup of tea, she may also describe the flavor of the tea as “ve very st y stron ong. g.”

  25. Elastic Intensity Scale We understand that she does not mean to suggest that her pain and the intensity of the tea flavor are the same. She means her pain was “very strong” in the context of all pains she has experienced and the tea flavor was “very strong” in the context of all teas she has sampled.

  26. Comparison errors caused by elasticity of our intensity scale • What happens when we treat our elastic scale as if it is absolute? • We first encountered this kind of error in our taste studies involving supertasters.

  27. REVE VERAL AL AR ARTIFAC ACT (s (show own b by y red da d dashe hed l d line nes) RE REALI LITY TY Very Ver y INCORRE NCORRECT A CT ASS SSUMP UMPTI TION ON Stro rong ng th that " t "Ver ery S Str tron ong Ta g Taste" ste" is th is the sa e same e Tast Ta ste absolu bsolute in te inten tensity sity for for n non onta taster sters (NT) a s (NT) and d su super perta taster sters (S s (ST) T) Qui uini nine ne Ver Very y Very Ver y Stro rong ng Suc ucros ose Stro rong ng Tast Ta ste Tast Ta ste Qui uini nine ne Na NaCl Suc ucros ose NaCl Na Othe Ot hers rs ST Ot Othe hers rs ST

  28. We are not the first to note this error. • Aitken, R. C. B. (1969). Measurement of feelings using visual analogue scales. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 62 , 989-993. • Narens, L., & Luce, R. D. (1983). How we may have been misled into believing in the interpersonal comparability of utility. Theory and Decision, 15 , 247-260. • Biernat, M., & Manis, M. (1994). Shifting standards and stereotype-based judgements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 , 5-20. • Birnbaum, M. H. (1999). How to show that 9>221: Collect judgements in a between-subjects design. Psychological Methods, 4 , 243-249.

  29. Illustration: PROBLEM and SOLUTION • Select two groups – people with many fungiform papillae: super ertaster ers – people with few fungiform papillae • Ask both groups to describe the sweetness of a coke – Both groups rate the sweetness to be about 2/3 of the distance from no sweet (0) to the strongest sweet they have ever tasted (100%). – Thus both groups seem to be experiencing the same sweetness. • Now ask both groups to match the sweetness to the loudness of a tone.

  30. Sweetness of a coke db subway 100 Supe pert rtasters rs train whistle 90 telephone dial tone Others 80 loud conversation 70 Each 10 db doubles loudness Correct conclusion: Supe upertas asters perceive twice as much sweetness as do Other ers.

  31. Summary of Magnitude Matching • Select a standard that is independent of the sensation we want to compare and ask subjects to rate the sensation of interest relative to the standard. • If the standard is roughly equal, on average, to two groups, then we can make an absolute comparison of the sensation of interest across the two groups. • This method was first used in taste (1975) and was formalized as “magnitude matching” in 1980 (Marks & Stevens).

  32. Let’s make this user friendly. • Let’s take the labeled magnitude scales we are used to (e.g., Natick 9-point scale). • Respace the intensity descriptors so that the scale has ratio properties (i.e., “8” denotes an intensity twice that of “4”).

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend