bargaining and coalition formation
play

Bargaining and Coalition Formation Dr James Tremewan - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Bargaining and Coalition Formation Dr James Tremewan (james.tremewan@univie.ac.at) Biology and Bargaining Biology and Bargaining Biology and Bargaining Heritability of Ultimatum Game Responder Behavior, Wallace et al (2007). The


  1. Bargaining and Coalition Formation Dr James Tremewan (james.tremewan@univie.ac.at) Biology and Bargaining

  2. Biology and Bargaining Biology and Bargaining • ”Heritability of Ultimatum Game Responder Behavior”, Wallace et al (2007). • The effect of testosterone on behaviour in the UG: • Burnham (2007). • Zak et al (2009). • Eisenegger et al (2010). • Zethraeus et al (2009). • ”Diminishing Reciprocal Fairness by Disrupting the Right Prefrontal Cortex”, Knoch et al (2006). 2/20

  3. Wallace et al (2007) Wallace et al (2007) • To what extent is responder behaviour 1 in the UG influenced by social/environmental and genetic factors? • Standard approach to answer this question is a Twin Design: • Monozygotic twins share identical genes; Dizygotic twins have imperfectly correlated genes. • Both types of twins share the same environment. • A greater correlation in behaviour between monozygotic twins than for dizygotic twins implies a genetic influence on behaviour. • Variation in behaviour can be broken down into genetic, common environmental, and non-shared environmental effects. • This study uses 71 DZ and 258 MZ pairs of twins from the Swedish Twin Registry at the Karolinska Institutet. • All subjects made decision as Proposer, then decisions whether to accept or reject every multiple of 10% (”strategy method”). 1 Almost no deviation from 50-50 split in proposer behaviour in Sweden. 3/20

  4. Wallace et al (2007) Wallace et al (2007): Findings No statistically significant difference between MZ and DZ acceptance thresholds. 4/20

  5. Wallace et al (2007) Wallace et al (2007): Findings continued • Spearman rank correlations: • Monozygotic twins: 0.39 • Dizygotic twins: -0.04 • Difference highly significant ( P < 0 . 01, two sided) • Point estimates of standard threshold model to decompose contributions to variation in behaviour: • Genetic contribution: 42% • Common environmental contribution: 0% • Non-shared environmental contribution: 58% • Conclusion: genetic influences are important determinants of rejection behaviour in the ultimatum game. 5/20

  6. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Burnham (2007): High-testosterone men and UG responder behaviour • Individual testosterone levels are under genetic influence - perhaps an explanation for Wallace et al findings? • Testosterone linked to agression, status seeking, reputation-management system, willingness to engage in conflict. • If men interpret low UG offers as a challenge, those with higher testosterone more likely to reject. • All subjects (N=26) made decision as Proposer, then decisions whether to accept or reject either possible offer ($5 or $25 out of $40). • Testosterone level measured from saliva on three non-experimental days at same time as UG decisions (14.00 - when rate of change of diurnal testosterone cycle is lowest). 6/20

  7. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Burnham (2007): Findings • Subjects who rejected $5 ( N = 6) had higher testosterone levels on average than those who accepted ( N = 20), P < 0 . 01. • Proposers offering $25 had higher testosterone levels on average than those offering $5, but not difference not significant. 7/20

  8. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Zak et al (2009) • Burnham (2007) identified correlation but not causation - need randomized manipulation of testosterone level. • Double-blind experiment, 25 men, within-subject design, pie=$10. • Testosterone administered using gel - blood tests to confirm increase in levels. • All subjects make decisions as Proposer and Receiver (acceptance threshold, i.e. strategy method). • Four decisions in each role with stranger matching. • They report tests on proposal levels, rejection threshold, and ”generosity” (defined as individual difference between proposal and rejection threshold). 8/20

  9. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Zak et al (2009): Findings and Comment • Average proposals lower when on T (T: $4.63, Placebo: $5.08, one-tailed paired t-test, N=200, p=0.001). • Rejection threshold higher when on T, but not significant (T: $3.05, Placebo: $2.92, one-tailed paired t-test, N=200, p=0.61). • Generosity lower when on T (T: $1.57, Placebo: $2.15, one-tailed paired t-test, N=200, p=0.035). • BUT: N=200?! No allowance for correlation between an individual’s decisions - seriously dodgy! • The author’s mention that there is variation in individuals’ decisions (average within-subjects SD of: proposals: $0.68; rejection threshold: $1.19; and generosity: $1.55). Not clear from paper if subjects received feedback between rounds: if so, also non-independence of observations between subjects in same session; if not, why the variation in decisions? 9/20

  10. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Eisenegger et al (2010) • Double-blind experiment, between-subject design, pie=10 Swiss Francs. • Subjects make decisions as either Proposer or Receiver • Offer either 5, 3, 2, or 0 Swiss Francs. • Receiver accepts or rejects (not strategy method). • Three decisions with stranger matching (no feedback). • Testosterone or placebo administered sub-lingually. • 121 female subjects (mean age=25 years). • Only female because required quantity and time for effect of treatment currently unknown for males • Subjects screened to ensure not pregnant, psychotic, etc. 10/20

  11. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Eisenegger et al (2010) • Competing hypotheses for Proposers: • ”Folk” hypothesis: T increases aggressive, egoistic behaviour (T lowers offers). • ”Social status” hypothesis: T increases concern for social status 2 and thus avoidance of rejection, which is a threat to status (T increases offers). • Also ask for belief about whether treated with T or placebo: • Social status hypothesis implies T increases offers. • Belief that one has been treated with T could be associated with lower offers if subjects believe Folk hypothesis (excuse for selfish behaviour?). 2 Which may be the underlying cause of aggressive behaviour, etc. 11/20

  12. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Eisenegger et al (2010): Results • Main results are from ANOVA model relating subject’s mean offer to actual treatment and belief. 3 • Subjects who received T made higher offers (T: $3.90, Placebo: $3.40, ANOVA, N=60, p=0.031). • Subjects who believed they received T made lower offers (bT: $3.08, bPlacebo: $3.88, ANOVA, N=60, p=0.006). • No significant correlation between treatment and beliefs, nor interaction effect. • No significant correlation effects on responder behaviour. 3 Justification for this model and further robustness tests are given in ”Supplementary Information”. 12/20

  13. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Zethraeus et al (2009) • Double-blind experiment, between-subject design, pie=SEK 400. • Subjects (203 Swedish post-menopausal women) were treated with testosterone, estrogen, or placebo for four weeks. • All subjects make decisions as Proposer and Receiver (acceptance threshold, i.e. strategy method). • Testosterone levels 4 times higher in treated group. • No significant effect found of testosterone (or estrogen) in UG behaviour 4 (or risk preferences, behaviour in a dictator and trust game). 4 Again very little deviation from 50-50 split in Proposer decision. 13/20

  14. Testosterone and the Ultimatum Game Discussion • Contradictory results in studies could result from: • Different experimental procedures (playing one or both roles, strategy method, etc.) • Different subject pools (testosterone may have different effects in males and females, which may depend on age). • Zethraeus et al (2009) conclude that: • Previously identified correlation between endogenous testosterone levels and behaviour are due to some other underlying biological factor. • Results may be spurious, with lack of countervailing evidence due to publication bias (i.e. 20 studies are run, and only one finds significance at %5 level, but this is the only ”interesting” result, so only one to be published.) 14/20

  15. Knoch et al (2006) Knoch et al (2006) • Sanfey et al (2003) found that the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of UG responders was activated more by unfair offers. • But is the R DLPFC crucial for determining response to fair/unfair behaviour? 15/20

  16. Knoch et al (2006) Knoch et al (2006) • Brain activity can be disrupted by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): Electric current passed through metal coil placed on scalp to produce magnetic field which affects firing of neurons. • Each subject (n=52) played as responder in UG 10 times in one of three treatments: rTMS of left DLPFC, rTMS of right DLPFC, or ”sham” rTMS. • Subjects also asked about perceived fairness of offers on a scale from 1 to 7. • In 10 games Proposer chooses offer; in 10 computer randomly chooses for Proposer - tests ”negative reciprocity” vs ”inequity aversion.” 16/20

  17. Knoch et al (2006) 17/20

  18. Knoch et al (2006) 18/20

  19. Knoch et al (2006) 19/20

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend