spatial vision
play

Spatial vision John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Spatial vision John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology NEUR3045 Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Today What is spatial vision? Physiology of spatial vision The dimensions of spatial vision: Fourier analysis


  1. Spatial vision John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology NEUR3045 Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1

  2. Today • What is spatial vision? • Physiology of spatial vision • The dimensions of spatial vision: Fourier analysis • Orientation • Adaptation and population coding • Spatial frequency • The contrast sensitivity function (CSF) • Population coding for spatial frequency • Foveal vs peripheral vision • Acuity and crowding 2

  3. What is spatial vision? • Our perception of the spatial V1 distribution of light across the visual field • The building blocks of object perception • The early stages of visual LGN processing Retina 3

  4. Spatial vision: LGN • Retinal ganglion cells and neurons in On-centre the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus have 
 centre-surround receptive fields - - + • Both on-centre and off-centre subtypes • Can highlight regions of change • (i.e. transitions from light to dark or 
 Off-centre dark to light) • But this does not give selectivity to - + + the orientation of edges 4

  5. Spatial vision: V1 • Hubel & Wiesel (1962) found - + + - + - - orientation selectivity in the + - - + + - + - - + + primary visual cortex (V1) - + - - LGN + + • Cells respond to particular V1 orientations of edges & lines • Have a preferred orientation that produces maximal spike/firing rate 
 (Schiller et al., 1976) • Likely built from particular combinations of centre-surround LGN neurons (Hubel & Wiesel, 1968) 5

  6. The dimensions of spatial vision • We can consider the ‘building blocks of spatial vision’ via Fourier analysis • Fourier (1822) showed that any signal can be decomposed into a sum of sine waves at different frequencies, amplitudes and phases 6

  7. Sine wave amplitude • Amplitude for a sine wave grating gives luminance contrast • The difference between light and dark regions in the scene 100% 75% 50% 25% 0% 100 50 0 7

  8. Sine wave phase • Phase determines the point at which variations occur in space, e.g. the starting point of the cycle • Represented in radians with a cyclical structure • Determines the position of edges in the scene π /2 3 π /2 2 π π 0 8

  9. Sine wave orientation • For two-dimensional images we also need to consider the orientation of the sine wave 0° 45° 90° 135° 180° • Orientation is certainly a key dimension for visual processing and we’ll return to this shortly 9

  10. Sine wave spatial frequency • Spatial frequency determines the variations across space • Reported as the number of cycles in a spatial region (peak to peak) • Captures the fine vs. coarse detail in an image 1 cyc/image 2 cyc/image 4 cyc/image 8 cyc/image 16 cyc/image Low High 10

  11. Summing the components • How do we make an image using sine waves? • Sum all of the component sine waves together • Easiest example: a square wave • How do you get a square wave from sine wave components? 11

  12. From to .. Fundamental (F) 3F 5F 7F • Take a sine wave Components with matched spatial frequency: the fundamental • Add the odd harmonics Fundamental (F) F+3F+5F+7F (increasing SF) F+3F F+3F+5F with decreasing amplitude Sum 12

  13. Summing the ideas • Fourier analysis tells us that we can break the visual scene down into component wave forms characterised by: • Amplitude (contrast) • Phase (position) • Spatial frequency (size) • Orientation (orientation…) • How are these dimensions encoded in the visual system? • Let’s look at two aspects: • Orientation • Spatial frequency 13

  14. Filtering an image with filters similar to the receptive fields of V1 cells gives us orientation energy at a range of spatial frequencies Spatial frequency (low to high) Orientation ∑ Original ∑ 14

  15. Local orientation • How do we encode orientation across the visual field? • One way to examine this: adaptation • Gibson (1937): prolonged viewing of one orientation reduces sensitivity to that orientation, and produces repulsion in the perceived tilt of dissimilar gratings Adapt Test 15

  16. Orientation adaptation • Adaptation reduces sensitivity to the adapting orientation • e.g. higher contrast required for detection • Can be attributed to reduced sensitivity of the underlying neurons s u s Pre-adapt Post-adapt u l u l u m m i t i S t S 16

  17. Local orientation • Gibson (1937): prolonged viewing of one orientation 
 reduces sensitivity to that orientation, and produces 
 repulsion in the perceived tilt of dissimilar gratings • i.e. adaptation reduces sensitivity 
 to the adapting orientation 
 (performance) • It can also alter the perceived 
 orientation (appearance), which 
 we call the tilt aftereffect Adapt Test 17

  18. Tilt aftereffect • Subsequent dissimilar orientations appear repulsed away • Produces a shift in the peak response away from the adaptor • Suggests population coding of orientation (Blakemore et al., 1971) Pre-adapt Post-adapt t r t s o s e e t T T p a d A shifted peak 18

  19. Two key principles • Adaptation • Reduces neural responses to continued stimulation and enhances responses to novel stimuli (redundancy reduction) • Population coding • Adapting to one orientation influences the perception of others • Our perception of orientation is inferred from the population of neural responses, e.g. as the peak • Allows a resolution higher than the sensitivity of individual neurons 19

  20. Orientation in context • Suppressive effects are also seen outside the local region • Contrast surround effects especially apparent with matched orientation & spatial frequency (Chubb et al., 1989) 20

  21. Surround suppression • Can again be attributed to reduced sensitivity of the underlying neurons, via connections from adjacent neurons s u s u l u l u m m i t i S t S 21

  22. Context effects: Tilt contrast • With dissimilar orientations can also see a shift in perceived orientation - similar to the effects of adaptation and the tilt aftereffect (Gibson, 1937) 22

  23. Tilt contrast • Adjacent orientations appear repulsed from one another • Also accounted for by shifts in population response, induced by adjacent neurons (Blakemore et al., 1970) d n u t t s o s e e r T T r u S 23

  24. Surround effects • Likely mediated by intracortical connections in V1 • Around 90% or neurons in V1 are suppressed by the activity of their neighbours (Jones et al , 2001) • Suppression minimises the response to homogeneous regions and highlights differences • Another instance of redundancy reduction (across space rather than time) • Minimises metabolic costs of neural firing (Laughlin et al., 1998) 24

  25. Spatial frequency • Fourier analysis also gives us a way to think about scale • Images contain information at different spatial frequencies • Which of these components is visible to an observer? • With Fourier analysis we can take a broader view of the image content that is visible to a given observer 1 cyc/image 2 cyc/image 4 cyc/image 8 cyc/image 16 cyc/image 25

  26. SF in natural scenes • What does spatial frequency mean for natural scenes? • Low-pass filtering: allow only the lowest SFs to be visible (broad blobby things) • High-pass filtering: allow only the highest SFs to be visible (edges & fine detail) 26

  27. A note on units • How do we characterise these spatial variations? • Cycles/image is OK for theoretical Fourier analyses • But for visual perception, image size on the retina is affected by both size and distance • Need to measure retinal size • Calculated as degrees of visual angle, where 
 tan( α ) = Height/Distance • For SF gives cycles/degree α 27

  28. Contrast sensitivity functions • Campbell & Robson (1968): high • Measured contrast sensitivity at a range of spatial frequencies Highest 
 • Contrast sensitivity function visible SF (CSF) peaks around 4 c/deg (acuity 
 cutoff) • Sensitivity is not greatest for 1 deg. uniform regions (low SF)! • Sensitivity also drops for high SFs - highest visible spatial low frequency is our acuity cutoff • Altogether defines our 
 ‘window of visibility’ 28

  29. A depiction of the CSF • We visualise low the CSF by plotting Contrast (amplitude) contrast against spatial frequency • Note: peak in the middle & the drop in visibility on high either side low high Spatial frequency 29

  30. What produces the CSF? • Why do we show this pattern of sensitivity? • Campbell & Robson (1968) hypothesised that the visual system is composed of spatial frequency channels - each sensitive to a restricted range of SFs • Blakemore & Campbell (1969) tested this using adaptation Adapt Test or Test 30

  31. CSF adaptation: predictions • Adaptation reduces sensitivity to contrast • But does it affect all SFs or just those of the adaptor? Single Channel Multiple Channels Adapting SF Adapting SF 31

  32. Multiple SF channels • Adaptation to a sine grating with 7.1 cycles per degree • Sensitivity is strongly reduced at the adapted SF and nearby values Adapting SF • No effect for SF values at the extremes of the range • Consistent with multiple channels for spatial frequency • Evidence that we separate the visual scene into its Fourier components (at least for SF) 32

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend