evidence informed health policy
play

Evidence-informed Health Policy: NASHP Pre-conference Origins of - PDF document

11/17/2017 October 23, 2017 Evidence-informed Health Policy: NASHP Pre-conference Origins of the EiHP Workshop Original workshops were developed as a joint project between CEbP and the Milbank Memorial Fund in 2009 Meant to equip


  1. 11/17/2017 October 23, 2017 Evidence-informed Health Policy: NASHP Pre-conference Origins of the EiHP Workshop • Original workshops were developed as a joint project between CEbP and the Milbank Memorial Fund in 2009 • Meant to equip policymakers in state and local government with knowledge, skills, and attitudes for understanding and applying research evidence • Adapted in 2014 to be delivered in different formats, timeframes, and audiences 1

  2. 11/17/2017 Acknowledgements • Martha Gerrity and Mark Gibson • Jane Beyer • Milbank Memorial Fund • NASHP and PCORI • Pew-MacArthur Results First Initiative To start • Introductions (of a sort) • Ground rules • Overview of the session 2

  3. 11/17/2017 Ground rules • Please don’t hesitate to ask questions • Three hours is a long time, so: – We’ll be generous with the breaks, including to get lunch at noon – We understand if you need to step out • Participation in the polls and other activities will make this more interesting and useful Overview of session • 10:30-10:45 Introduction, ground rules, overview • 10:45-11:25 Defining EiHP and Evidence basics • 11:25-11:30 Break • 11:30-12:00 Summarizing evidence and Common pitfalls • 12:00-12:20 Break for lunch • 12:20-1:10 Examples from the evidence • 1:10-1:30 Wrap-up, discussion, and questions 3

  4. 11/17/2017 What is evidence? • For our purposes, evidence comes from research that: – Is intended to test the validity of a claim – Uses reproducible methods – Collects and interprets data using tests to distinguish between chance and true effects – Can be scrutinized by peers and the public – Falsifiable! • More simply stated by W. Edwards Deming: “In God we trust. All others must bring data.” What is EiHP? • An approach to health policy decisions that is informed by the best and most complete available research evidence • A structured way to use research to better understand what works, recognizing that: – Not all studies are created equal – Some studies may not be relevant to policymaking – Transparency in identifying and applying studies is important 4

  5. 11/17/2017 What is EiHP? • Systematic process in which relevant research is: – Identified in an unbiased fashion – Critically interpreted to understand its quality – Combined to provide a better estimate of the real effects – Applied appropriately to policymaking – Re-assessed when new information becomes available Why should I use EiHP? • When tackling complex issues, it’s useful to have a sense of: – What we know – What we don’t know – What we merely believe 5

  6. 11/17/2017 Why should I use EiHP? • An analogy from medicine – When there is a delay in adopting an effective therapy or discarding an ineffective or harmful therapy, lives are at stake – This hazard is magnified when we consider programs and policies that affect the lives of many more people Why should I use EiHP? • EiHP improves the chances that a policy or investment will achieve the desired ends – And reduces the likelihood that a failed policy will have to be abandoned in the future • EiHP can be a starting point for engaging stakeholders with divergent views • When done deliberately and transparently, EiHP can increase public confidence in the policymaking process • Consider the alternatives: – Anecdotes – Opinion – Intuition 6

  7. 11/17/2017 Understanding Evidence The origins of epidemiology 7

  8. 11/17/2017 The origins of epidemiology… and public health policy https://upload.wikimedia.org/wikipedia/commons/c/cb/John_Snow_memorial_and_pub.jpg Health in the 20 th Century • Vaccination • Safer, healthier foods • Motor-vehicle safety • Maternal and prenatal care • Safer workplaces • Family planning • Control of infectious • Fluoridation disease • Decline in deaths • Tobacco control from heart attack and policies stroke CDC. (1999). MMWR, 48(12):241-243. 8

  9. 11/17/2017 Asking the right question • Having a standard way for framing questions you hope to answer with the evidence is critical – A research tool for assessing which studies are relevant – An exercise in establishing, a priori, what types of research and outcomes would influence the policy decision being contemplated – Agreement about desired outcomes and a process for reviewing and summarizing the evidence can help build consensus Asking the right question • PICO(TS+) – Population – Intervention – Comparison – Outcome – (Timing, Setting, Policy context) 9

  10. 11/17/2017 Asking the right question: Population • Demographics • Conditions • Geography Asking the right question: Intervention • Drug, device, or procedure • Diagnostic test • New methods of organizing or delivering care • Systems or process changes • Policy changes 10

  11. 11/17/2017 Asking the right questions: Comparison • Status quo • Placebo • Sham procedure • Alternate treatment Asking the right question: Outcomes • Health or wellbeing (most important outcomes) • Surrogate measures • System performance • Process measures 11

  12. 11/17/2017 Asking the right question: PICO example Population: Adults with serious mental illness Intervention: Assertive community treatment Comparator: Usual care Outcomes: Psychiatric hospitalization, emergency dept use, homelessness, psychiatric symptoms, medication adherence The “reverse” PICO • When there are disagreements about the meaning or applicability of a study, it can help to reverse the PICO process – May clarify whether the study is really answering the question you are interested in 12

  13. 11/17/2017 The Challenge of Using Evidence • There are an estimated 24 million studies in PubMed, each a potential piece of evidence • Studies often reach conflicting results • It’s easy to pick and choose the evidence that best supports a given position • How do you know what evidence is most accurate and reliable? Why are some studies “good” and some studies “bad”? • Some studies are not designed to fairly answer the question they pose • Studies can be biased to favor certain results, intentionally or unintentionally • Conflict of interest can result in a bias toward favorable results • It’s time consuming and takes some technical sophistication to sort through studies to assess quality and summarize results 13

  14. 11/17/2017 The essence of epidemiology • How do you explore the relationship between an exposure and an outcome ? – Hypothesize and observe – Hypothesize and experiment The evidence hierarchy Murad et al. (2016). Evidence-Based Medicine Published Online First: 23 June 2016. doi:10.1136/ebmed-2016-110401 14

  15. 11/17/2017 The evidence hierarchy Murad et al. (2016). Evidence-Based Medicine Published Online First: 23 June 2016. doi:10.1136/ebmed-2016-110401 Case series or reports • Simply describes a set of cases and their outcomes • Often used for rare conditions, or when a treatment or test is very new • Usually represent the experience of a single center • Should not be used to establish effectiveness of a treatment • Be especially wary of non-consecutive case series (meaning that the authors picked out only the cases they wanted to describe) 15

  16. 11/17/2017 The risk of case reports • Porter and Jick, 1981: • Addiction is rare in people treated with narcotics The risk of case reports Leung, et al. (2017) NEJM. 376;22. 16

  17. 11/17/2017 Case-control studies Exposed Cases Unexposed Exposed Controls Unexposed Past Start of study Case-control example Large class High test scores Small class Large class Low test scores Small class Past Start of study 17

  18. 11/17/2017 Case-control studies • Advantages: – Quick and inexpensive – Particularly good for investigating rare outcomes – Dynamic populations • Disadvantages: – Recall bias – Cases and controls may not be representative (selection bias) – Confounding Prospective cohort studies Exposed Outcomes Healthy Population Unexposed Outcomes Start of study Future 18

  19. 11/17/2017 Prospective cohort example Small class Test scores Healthy Population Large class Test scores Start of study Future Prospective cohort studies • Advantages: – Eliminate recall bias – Can examine multiple outcomes – Allows estimation of incidence of outcome – Better at detecting long-term harms than other studies • Disadvantages – Expensive and take a long time – Loss to follow-up (attrition bias) – Still subject to confounding 19

  20. 11/17/2017 Retrospective cohort studies Exposed Outcomes Unexposed Outcomes Start of study Past Retrospective cohort example Small class Test scores Large class Test scores Start of study Past 20

  21. 11/17/2017 Retrospective cohort studies • Advantages – Much easier to do than prospective cohorts • Disadvantages – Only as good as the data set being used • Claims data and diagnosis codes are often unreliable – Data about confounding factors could be missing and can’t be adjusted for Randomized controlled trial (RCT) Intervention Outcomes Population of Interest Control Outcomes Start of study Future 21

Download Presentation
Download Policy: The content available on the website is offered to you 'AS IS' for your personal information and use only. It cannot be commercialized, licensed, or distributed on other websites without prior consent from the author. To download a presentation, simply click this link. If you encounter any difficulties during the download process, it's possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

Recommend


More recommend